Back to Journals » Psychology Research and Behavior Management » Volume 17

The Association Between Intolerance of Uncertainty and Mobile Phone Addiction Among Overseas Chinese Students During COVID-19: The Mediating Roles of Perceived Stress and Rumination

Authors Sang Z , Chen HF , Yeung JWK , Xu L

Received 11 March 2024

Accepted for publication 23 May 2024

Published 3 July 2024 Volume 2024:17 Pages 2573—2585

DOI https://doi.org/10.2147/PRBM.S463399

Checked for plagiarism Yes

Review by Single anonymous peer review

Peer reviewer comments 2

Editor who approved publication: Dr Gabriela Topa



Ziyang Sang, Hui-Fang Chen, Jerf WK Yeung, Leilei Xu

Department of Social and Behavioural Sciences, City University of Hong Kong, Kowloon, Hong Kong Special Administrative Region

Correspondence: Hui-Fang Chen, Department of Social and Behavioural Sciences, City University of Hong Kong, 83 Tat Chee Ave, Kowloon, Hong Kong Special Administrative Region, Tel +852 3442297, Fax +852 34420283, Email [email protected]

Purpose: This study investigated the mechanism underlying the association between intolerance of uncertainty and mobile phone addiction among Chinese overseas students during the COVID-19 pandemic by examining the mediating roles of perceived stress and rumination.
Patients and Methods: An online questionnaire survey was distributed via social media platforms popular in mainland China. The items collected demographic information and assessed intolerance of uncertainty, perceived stress, rumination, and mobile phone addiction. A total of 249 respondents completed the questionnaire.
Results: The findings suggest a considerably high risk of mobile phone addiction in the study period among overseas Chinese students, as three-fourths of the participants may have been susceptible to mobile phone addiction according to the suggested cut-off point of the Chinese version of the Smartphone Addiction Scale–Short Form. Intolerance of uncertainty showed a significant positive direct effect on mobile phone addiction. The mediation analyses suggest that intolerance of uncertainty affected mobile phone addiction mainly through three pathways: the mediating effect of perceived stress, the mediating effect of rumination, and the chain mediating effect of perceived stress and rumination.
Conclusion: This study enhances understanding of mobile phone addiction among Chinese overseas students and suggests the mediating roles of rumination and perceived stress in the relationship between intolerance of uncertainty and mobile phone addiction. The study also provides suggestions for interventions among Chinese students overseas.

Keywords: vulnerability, mental health, health issues, internet

Introduction

The onset of the COVID-19 pandemic heralded a trend of remote work, study, and socializing that resulted in electronic devices, such as mobile phones, playing a more central role in people’s lives.1 Most people use mobile phones not only as communication tools for social contact but also as devices to boost productivity or meet their entertainment needs.2 The frequency and duration of mobile phone use are rising, becoming risk factors for mobile phone addiction.3–5

Mobile phone addiction is characterized by excessive and uncontrolled use of mobile devices and has consequences for individual health and development, such as depression and anxiety, sleep quality, and academic achievement.2,6–10 Although research on mobile phone addiction is well documented, less research has been conducted in the context of challenging external environments, such as during COVID-19, which caused considerable uncertainty and stress worldwide. This paper investigates how the mechanisms of influence of mobile phone addiction act in an external environment that is unpredictable, ambiguous, and filled with negative information and multiple stressors. Specifically, we explore how intolerance of uncertainty (IU) influences mobile phone addiction, identify potential mediating variables, and develop sequential mediation models to evaluate all factors.

Intolerance of Uncertainty and Mobile Phone Addiction

COVID-19 had catastrophic effects on the global economy as well as on human safety and mental health.11,12 The constant emergence of health risks due to the pandemic and the consequent social restrictions of residential lockdowns and enforced social distancing put individuals’ mental health in a state of flux, with any sudden uncertainty leading to ups and downs in mood.13 The high level of uncertainty regarding social circumstances shows great impact on individuals’ lives.14

Individuals respond differently to the uncertainty of events, which manifests as the stable personality trait known as IU, defined as the tendency to react negatively to an uncertain event or situation independently of its probability of occurrence and its associated consequences.15,16 People with high levels of IU may experience negative emotions as a result of an event or a situation with uncertain outcomes or situation, reacting negatively to the uncertainty or even developing dysfunctional responses.17,18 Most individuals with high levels of IU are extraordinarily concerned with the external environment and highly susceptible to perceiving external information as a threat and developing negative beliefs.19 As a result, IU is a vulnerability factor in several psychiatric disorders. For instance, it is highly associated with generalized anxiety disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder, and health anxiety.17,20–22

IU is also a risk factor for many problematic behaviors, for example, alcohol abuse and eating disorders.23–25 Several related studies have shown that IU also has a significant effect on internet and mobile phone addiction, for instance.2,13 According to the model of compensatory internet use theory, people may spend a substantial amount of time online to escape from real-life problems and/or to mitigate the effects of negative emotions; for them, the internet becomes a sort of compensation for stress and adverse events.26 On the positive side, mobile phone use allows individuals to avoid the reality that distresses them and compensates for their immediate psychological needs; on the negative side, individuals become overly dependent on the internet to the point of abuse and addiction, leaving troubling life problems and challenges unsolved.27,28 Therefore, we propose the following:

H1: IU positively predicts mobile phone addiction.

Perceived Stress as a Mediator Between IU and Mobile Phone Addiction

The degree to which people’s lives are unpredictable, uncontrollable, and overwhelmed determines perceived stress, and related research has demonstrated that IU affects individuals’ perceptions of stress.29,30 Carleton31 identifies information processing bias as an important factor in the relationship between IU and anxiety disorders, with IU inducing a bias to classify unspecified stimuli as threatening, which results in the brain being more likely to perceive tension when processing uncertain information. In other words, those with a high IU are more prone to experiencing ambiguous stress and becoming anxious,17,31,32 reflecting that IU is a vulnerability factor for perceived stress.33 Previous studies also have found a positive correlation between IU and perceived stress and have indicated that IU may increase the negative impact of stressors and predict mental health problems, which in turn exacerbate perceived stress.30,34

In addition, recent studies have demonstrated that perceived stress is highly connected with addictive behaviors and negative emotional states, such as anxiety and depression, and that mobile phone addiction is similarly influenced by perceived stress.35,36 According to general strain theory, negative experiences from various stresses or strains are at the root of problematic behaviors.37 From this perspective, mobile phone addiction is a by-product of coping with stressful pain and strains.38 Several empirical studies have also demonstrated a significant positive effect of perceived stress on mobile phone addiction.39 Due to the pandemic, people’s perceived stress was influenced not only by their previous life, learning, and employment experiences but also by the uncertainty caused by COVID-19. Uncertainty heightens perceptions of stressful life events, causing people to flee or seek solace, hence fostering an increase in mobile phone addiction.40 This leads to the study’s second hypothesis:

H2: IU positively predicts perceived stress, which in turn positively predicts mobile phone addiction, meaning that perceived stress mediates the relationship between IU and mobile phone addiction.

Stress, Rumination, and Mobile Phone Addiction

Rumination is a form of maladaptive negative thinking that is manifested primarily when people experience distress and focus constantly and repeatedly on negative situations and their consequences rather than on actively solving the problem; it can lead to a persistent state of depression.41,42 When individuals engage in rumination, they draw negative emotional experiences from their memories, and these negative or emotionally charged emotions encourage them to recall the memories repeatedly.43,44 Negative emotions are thus the defining characteristic of rumination.45 During the COVID-19 pandemic, people were unable to predict when negative events and information would occur, and the changing external environment contributed to individual psychological confusion, which also prompted people to repeatedly contemplate and even exaggerate risks, generating increased fear, depression, and anxiety.14,46,47 As mentioned earlier, IU is associated with cognitive biases, and it may prompt rumination48 and then a reliance on mobile phones to de-stress from rumination, inspiring the third hypothesis:

H3: IU positively predicts rumination, which in turn positively predicts mobile phone addiction. In other words, rumination mediates the relationship between IU and mobile phone addiction.

The cognitive-behavioral model indicates that stressful life events are the distal cause of pathological internet use, whereas the proximal cause and central factor is maladaptive cognition.49 Mobile phone addiction shares many qualities with pathological internet use, and the cognitive-behavioral model has been widely applied to the field of mobile phone addiction.40 Rumination working with perceived stress as a distal factor leads to maladaptive cognitions, so those prone to rumination may develop more severe mobile phone addiction.7 This leads to our fourth hypothesis:

H4: Perceived stress positively predicts rumination, which in turn positively predicts mobile phone addiction. Rumination mediates the relationship between perceived stress and mobile phone addiction.

If the first and fourth hypotheses are supported by the data, they suggest that high IU will lead to greater stress, leading to frequent rumination and then frequent mobile phone use, inspiring the fifth research hypothesis:

H5: Perceived stress and rumination sequentially mediate the relationship between IU and mobile phone addiction.

The Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model may be useful in illustrating the overall relationship between these variables.50 The I-PACE consists of two main processes and several significant factors, such as the individual’s core characteristics, subjective perceptions of the situation, cognitive and emotional reactions to the situation, and decision-making behavior. In the first stage, IU (the core characteristic) influences a person’s perception of stress (subjective perception) and rumination (cognitive response) during a COVID-19 outbreak (situational factor), compelling them to direct their attention to the mobile phone (decision-making). Using mobile phones, in turn, influences their assessment and decision-making processes. In the next stage, this process is repeated, eventually leading to mobile phone addiction.

To preclude confounding effects in this study, the demographic covariates of participants’ gender, age, educational level, and study region were controlled in the modeling, as prior research has reported their impacts on cognitive and emotional differences and mobile phone use behaviors.8,13,18,50

Materials and Methods

Participants

Oversea Chinese students experiencing COVID-19 were the target population in this study. All the Participants were 18 years of age or older. Based on previous literature,51 the sample sizes of a multiple-step multiple mediation model normally range from 221 to 1000. Accordingly, we collected 249 valid questionnaires, which power analysis indicated was sufficient.52

Measures

Intolerance of Uncertainty

To assess IU, we used the Chinese version of the 12-item Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale (IUS-12), adapted from the Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale.15,18,53 These 12 items reflect two dimensions—anticipatory anxiety and inhibitory anxiety—and are rated on a 5-point scale in which 1 indicates “Not at all characteristic of me” and 5 represents “Entirely characteristic of me”. Higher scores indicate less tolerance for uncertainty. A sample item is “A small unforeseen event can spoil everything, even with the best of planning”. Wu et al’s study reports Cronbach’s alpha values above 0.7 on the two dimensions and the whole scale of the Chinese IUS-12, indicating satisfactory reliability and validity.53 In our study, the Cronbach’s α coefficient was 0.86.

Mobile Phone Addiction

To assess mobile phone addiction, we used the Chinese version of the Smartphone Addiction Scale–Short Version.54,55 It includes 10 items, such as “Feeling impatient and fretful when I am not holding my smartphone”, and is rated on a 6-point scale from 1 (“totally disagree”) to 6 (“totally agree”). Higher scores represent more severe mobile phone addiction. Lopez-Fernandez suggests a total score higher than 33 for women and over 31 for men to indicate mobile phone addiction.56 Zhao et al report good reliability (Cronbach’s α = 0.90) and validity among Chinese college students.57 A Cronbach’s α of 0.89 was obtained for this scale in our study.

Perceived Stress

The Chinese version of the Perceived Stress Scale was used to assess perceived stress.29,58 The scale contains 14 items, including “In the last month, how often have you been upset because of something that happened unexpectedly?” A 5-point Likert scale from 1 (“never”) to 5 (“very often”) was used, and a higher score indicated higher perceived stress. The scale has shown good reliability (Cronbach’s α = 0.87) and good construct validity with Chinese college students.59 A Cronbach’s α of 0.70 was obtained in this study.

Rumination

The Chinese version of the Ruminative Response Scale-10 was used to assess rumination.44,60,61 Its 10 items reflect the two dimensions, reflection and brooding, and are rated on a 4-point scale (1 = “almost never”; 4 = “almost always”). A sample item is “Think ‘What am I doing to deserve this’”. The higher the score, the more likely one is to ruminate. A study supported the use of this scale among Chinese undergraduates, with a value of 0.75 for Cronbach’s α.62 A coefficient of 0.95 was obtained in this study. The participants were also asked to provide information on gender, age, educational level, study region, and current location.

Procedure

This study complies with the Declaration of Helsinki. The authors obtained ethical approval from the research ethics subcommittee of City University of Hong Kong (reference number: SSB5790-202203-15) before data collection. A quick-response code (QR code) to the electronic questionnaire and invitation was posted on social media platforms popular in mainland China, such as WeChat, Douban, and Xiaohongshu. Eligible overseas Chinese students (ie, 18 years old or above) accessed the questionnaire via the QR code. On the information page, all the participants were informed of their rights (eg, voluntary participation without any incentives and freedom to withdraw from the study at any time without penalty), confidentiality and anonymity, and benefits and risks. They were also informed that clicking on the “next page/continue” button implied their agreement to participate in the research with an understanding of all stated information, voluntary participation, and being 18 years of age or older. The data collection period extended from April 20 to May 20, 2022, on the Wenjuanxing survey platform. The participants usually spent around 10 minutes completing the online questionnaire.

Data Analysis

Descriptive statistics and group comparisons were conducted using SPSS 27.0 (IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY, 2020). A series of independent t-tests was conducted to compare group differences in IU, mobile phone addiction, and rumination to determine whether gender, study region, or age impacted the studied variables. Pearson correlation analysis was conducted to explore relationships between paired variables, controlling for the impact of gender, age, educational level, and study region.

Mplus 8.9 was used to examine the mediating effect of perceived stress and rumination on the relationship between IU and mobile phone addiction, controlling for the impact of gender, age, educational level, and study region. Fit indexes, including chi-square tests, the Steiger-Lind root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and the Bentler comparative fit index (CFI) and standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) were used to test the data model fit.63–65 After the first four research hypotheses were found to be supported, a sequential mediating model was tested.

Results

Demographic Information

Table 1 shows the participants’ backgrounds. A total of 249 participants completed the questionnaire, including 53 males (21.2%) and 196 females (78.7%). Two-thirds of the participants were aged 23 years or above, 67.4% possessed a master’s degree, and 70.2% were currently studying in Hong Kong, Macao, or Taiwan. Notably, 189 participants (75.9%) were susceptible to mobile phone addiction, as their mobile phone addiction scores exceeded the cut point.

Table 1 Descriptive Statistics of Overseas Chinese Students

Independent sample t-tests indicate that the age group of 23 years and above reported significantly lower IU than the younger group (t = −2.77, p < 0.01). Participants with a bachelor’s degree reported statistically significantly higher rumination (t = −1.99, p < 0.05) and mobile phone addiction (t = −2.08, p < 0.05) than those with a master’s or higher degree.

Hypothesis Tests

Table 2 lists the means, standard deviations, and correlations of the studied variables, controlled for the impact of gender, age, educational level, and study region. All the variables were statistically positively correlated with one another (rs > 0.26, ps < 0.01). H1, “IU positively predicts mobile phone addiction”, is supported.

Table 2 Partial Correlations Between Variables

Figure 1 shows the findings for H2. Perceived stress mediated the relationship between IU and mobile phone addiction (χ2 = 7.99; p = 0.09; RMSEA = 0.06; CFI = 0.96; SRMR = 0.05) after the impacts of gender, age, educational level, and place of study on mobile phone addiction were controlled. The regression coefficient from IU to mobile phone addiction was not significant after adding perceived stress (β = 0.21, p = 0.07), indicating a mediating role of perceived stress in the relationship between IU and mobile phone addiction.

Figure 1 The mediating model of perceived stress in the relationship between intolerance of uncertainty and mobile phone addiction. **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001.

Figure 2 presents the findings for H3. Rumination was a significant mediator of the relationship between IU and mobile phone addiction (χ2 = 1.74; p = 0.78; RMSEA < 0.001; CFI = 1.0; SRMR = 0.02) after we controlled for the influence of gender, age, educational level, and school area. IU did not exhibit a direct effect on mobile phone addiction after adding rumination to the model (β = 0.05, p = 0.67). The hypothetical mediating role of rumination is supported (H3).

Figure 2 The mediating model of rumination in the relationship between intolerance of uncertainty and mobile phone addiction. ***p < 0.001.

Rumination was also a significant mediator of the relationship between stress and mobile phone addiction (χ2 = 4.83; p = 0.31; RMSEA = 0.03; CFI = 0.99; SRMR = 0.03) after we controlled for the influence of gender, age, educational level, and school area (Figure 3). Stress did not exhibit a direct effect on mobile phone addiction after adding rumination to the model (β = 0.41, p = 0.09). Rumination completely mediated the relationship between stress and mobile phone addiction. H4 is supported.

Figure 3 The mediating model of rumination in the relationship between perceived stress and mobile phone addiction. **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001.

We went further to examine H5 (Figure 4). The fit indexes suggested good model data fit (χ2 = 11.04; p = 0.20; RMSEA = 0.04; CFI = 0.99; SRMR = 0.04) and that the impact of IU on mobile phone addiction would go through perceived stress and then rumination, while part of IU impact would directly go through rumination. There was no significant prediction from perceived stress to mobile phone addiction (β = 0.41, p = 0.08). H5 is supported.

Figure 4 The sequential mediating model. **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001.

Discussion

The present study integrated general strain theory, compensatory internet use theory, and the I-PACE model to understand the mechanism underlying the relationship between IU and mobile phone addiction. We examined if IU was associated with mobile phone addiction among overseas Chinese students during COVID-19 and the mediated role and a chain mediation model of perceived stress and rumination in the relationship between IU and mobile phone addiction.26,37,50 The Results show that the four studied variables were positively correlated. Both perceived stress and rumination were significant mediators when IU was used to predict mobile phone addiction. Further analysis indicated that the impact from IU went directly through rumination to predict mobile phone addiction and that part of IU’s impact passed through perceived stress and then rumination to influence mobile phone addiction.

Compensatory internet use theory suggests that excessive mobile phone use is a compensatory strategy for mitigating individuals’ decreased distress tolerance and increased anxiety induced by environmental factors.26 Based on this theory, when individuals face fear and uncertainty from the environment (eg, during COVID-19), they are prone to use mobile phones as a compensatory strategy to relieve restlessness and anxiety.26 In particular, individuals with high IU would be more inclined to use mobile phones as a compensatory strategy, eventually leading to mobile phone addiction.2 Consistent with compensatory internet use theory and previous studies,66,67 our study found a positive relationship between IU and mobile phone addiction. We also found that around 76% of our participants potentially had a mobile phone addiction problem.2 In other words, higher IU among overseas Chinese students may drive higher mobile phone engagement and increase vulnerability to mobile phone addiction, meriting further investigations of the mechanism underlying the link between IU and mobile phone addiction. The present study narrowed a knowledge gap by examining the mediating effects of perceived stress and rumination.

Before testing the constructed model, we investigated the relationship between the predictor and the mediator, namely perceived stress. Previous research has indicated that IU is associated with negative cognitive outcomes, such as hopelessness, which is a significant predictor of perceived stress among college students.68–70 We might speculate that individuals with high IU were more prone to worry about the presence of stressors (eg, whether they would get COVID-19) during the pandemic, which in turn triggered feelings of uncertainty and increased their perceived stress.39 Two studies on university students in Turkey have shown that IU may cause procrastination and affect the proactive career behaviors of these students and can bring about negative consequences, such as increased perceived stress.71,72 Furthermore, during COVID-19, overseas college students experienced severe stress and depression due to the loss of normal campus life and interpersonal communications.73 Oversea Chinese students may have faced severe pressure because of the prejudice against China after the outbreak of COVID-19.74 Thus, according to general strain theory, feeling hopeless against real-life pressure might have impelled them to escape reality by using mobile phones, eventually developing mobile phone addiction. Individuals with high COVID-19 IU would have felt more stressed, leading to a higher risk of mobile phone addiction.37

The study also suggests a mediating role of rumination between IU and mobile phone addiction. People with high IU are more prone to develop negative thinking patterns, such as overestimating risk, and habitually ruminative people are more susceptible to such pessimistic predictions.75,76 Also, the students’ cognitive adjustment (such as rumination) was related to excessive mobile phone use.77 Consistent with previous studies, we found that people with high IU were more likely to ruminate and that rumination may further aggravate mobile phone addiction.

The present study adopted the I-PACE model to propose a sequential mediator model to enhance understanding of mobile phone addiction, examining the chain mediating effect of perceived stress and rumination on the relationship between IU and mobile phone addiction.50 As anticipated, the predicted effect of IU on mobile phone addiction among Chinese international students was initially mediated by both perceived stress and rumination. The direct effect of IU on mobile phone addiction was not significant after including perceived stress and rumination in the model. Also, rumination completely mediated the association between perceived stress and mobile phone addiction.50 In other words, mobile phone addiction can be explained by the interaction effect between personal (uncertainty intolerance), cognitive (stress perception and rumination), and executive (coping style) factors in the I-PACE model.50 It is concluded that, during the COVID-19 pandemic, individuals with high IU may have been more likely to experience stress than those with low IU, which made them more prone to ruminate, thus exacerbating their use of mobile phones as an avoidance strategy and increasing the likelihood of mobile phone addiction.

Contributions

The present study not only contributes to theoretical understanding of the phenomenon of mobile phone addiction but also has practical implications. The proposed chain mediation model provides a basis for guiding college students to weaken mobile phone addiction caused by an uncertain environment and to use mobile phones rationally. As previous research has shown that social support is effective in coping with perceived stress, Measures such as group counseling could provide social support to college students to reduce their perceived stress, leading to less frequent use of mobile phones.78 In addition, cognitive-behavioral therapy could be offered to reduce college students’ rumination so as to mitigate mobile phone addiction.

Limitations

A few Limitations in this study must be addressed. First, the convenience sampling method may restrict the generalizability of the results. Most participants were studying in Hong Kong, Macao, or Taiwan, and their experiences may have differed from those of overseas students in North America or Europe. In addition, children and teenagers are more prone to mobile phone addiction than young adults.79,80 Therefore, future research could validate our findings by increasing the sample size and age range of participants.

Second, as a cross-sectional study, this research revealed only correlations between variables and not causality. In future research, longitudinal designs and clinical trials may be employed to clarify the temporal and causal associations between the variables.

Third, an online questionnaire and self-reported measurements were used in this study to measure mobile phone addiction. However, most people underestimate their mobile phone usage and thus report less time and frequency of mobile phone use.81 Future research may try to use objective measurements, such as screen time recorded by mobile phone systems, to record mobile phone usage more accurately.

Conclusion

This study investigated how IU was associated with mobile phone addiction among Chinese students overseas during COVID-19. A positive association between IU and mobile phone addiction was confirmed, and mediating roles of perceived stress and rumination in this relationship were found. We further conducted a sequential mediating model and found that IU would increase perceived stress, leading to increased rumination and a stronger tendency of frequent use of a mobile phone. Practitioners and educators could consider offering training on coping strategies (eg, how to reduce stress and restructure thinking patterns) for students with high IU. Such training might help to reduce students’ dependence on their mobile phone, thus preventing problematic usage.

Funding

This work was supported by the Hong Kong University Grants Committee, General Research Fund [CityU 11608721].

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

1. Espitia A, Mattoo A, Rocha N, Ruta M, Winkler D. Pandemic trade: COVID-19, remote work and global value chains. World Econ. 2022;45(2):561–589. doi:10.1111/twec.13117

2. Rozgonjuk D, Elhai JD, Täht K, Vassil K, Levine JC, Asmundson GJG. Non-social smartphone use mediates the relationship between intolerance of uncertainty and problematic smartphone use: evidence from a repeated-measures study. Comput Human Behav. 2019;96:56–62. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2019.02.013

3. Mannion KH, Nolan SA. The effect of smartphones on anxiety: an attachment issue or fear of missing out? Cogent Psychol. 2020;7:1869378.

4. Savci M, Tekin A, Elhai JD. Prediction of problematic social media use (PSU) using machine learning approaches. Curr Psychol. 2022;41(5):2755–2764. doi:10.1007/s12144-020-00794-1

5. Nikhita CS, Jadhav PR, Ajinkya SA. Prevalence of mobile phone dependence in secondary school adolescents. J Clin Diagn Res. 2015;9(11):VC06- VC09. doi:10.7860/JCDR/2015/14396.6803

6. Kim JH, Seo M, David P. Alleviating depression only to become problematic mobile phone users: can face-to-face communication be the antidote? Comput Human Behav. 2015;51:440–447. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2015.05.030

7. Elhai JD, Dvorak RD, Levine JC, Hall BJ. Problematic smartphone use: a conceptual overview and systematic review of relations with anxiety and depression psychopathology. J Affect Disord. 2017;207:251–259. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2016.08.030

8. Demirci K, Akgönül M, Akpinar A. Relationship of smartphone use severity with sleep quality, depression, and anxiety in university students. J Behav Addict. 2015;4(2):85–92. doi:10.1556/2006.4.2015.010

9. Ng KC, Wu LH, Lam HY, et al. The relationships between mobile phone use and depressive symptoms, bodily pain, and daytime sleepiness in Hong Kong secondary school students. Addict Behav. 2020;101:105975. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2019.04.033

10. Seo DG, Park Y, Kim MK, Park J. Mobile phone dependency and its impacts on adolescents’ social and academic behaviors. Comput Human Behav. 2016;63:282–292. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2016.05.026

11. Garfin DR, Silver RC, Holman EA. The novel coronavirus (COVID-2019) outbreak: amplification of public health consequences by media exposure. Health Psychol. 2020;39(5):355–357. doi:10.1037/hea0000875

12. Schimmenti A, Billieux J, Starcevic V. The four horsemen of fear: an integrated model of understanding fear experiences during the COVID-19 pandemic. Clin Neuropsychiatry. 2020;17(2):41–45. doi:10.36131/CN20200202

13. Luo R, Li Q, Meng G, et al. The association between intolerance of uncertainty and internet addiction during the second wave of the coronavirus disease 2019 pandemic: a multiple mediation model considering depression and risk perception. PsyCh J. 2022;11(3):383–391. doi:10.1002/pchj.545

14. Satici B, Saricali M, Satici SA, Griffiths MD. Intolerance of uncertainty and mental wellbeing: serial mediation by rumination and fear of COVID-19. Int J Ment Health Addict. 2020;2020:1.

15. Carleton RN, Norton MA, Asmundson GJ. Fearing the unknown: a short version of the Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale. J Anxiety Disord. 2007;21(1):105–117. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.03.014

16. Ladouceur R, Gosselin P, Dugas MJ. Experimental manipulation of intolerance of uncertainty: a study of a theoretical model of worry. Behav Res Ther. 2000;38(9):933–941. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00133-3

17. Buhr K, Dugas MJ. Investigating the construct validity of intolerance of uncertainty and its unique relationship with worry. J Anxiety Disord. 2006;20(2):222–236. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2004.12.004

18. Freeston MH, Rhéaume J, Letarte H, Dugas MJ, Ladouceur R. Why do people worry? Pers Individ Dif. 1994;17(6):791–802. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(94)90048-5

19. Sexton KA, Dugas MJ. Defining distinct negative beliefs about uncertainty: validating the factor structure of the Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale. Psychol Assess. 2009;21(2):176–186. doi:10.1037/a0015827

20. McEvoy PM, Hyett MP, Shihata S, Price JE, Strachan L. The impact of methodological and measurement factors on transdiagnostic associations with intolerance of uncertainty: a meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. 2019;73:101778. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2019.101778

21. Jacoby RJ, Fabricant LE, Leonard RC, Riemann BC, Abramowitz JS. Just to be certain: confirming the factor structure of the Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Anxiety Disord. 2013;27(5):535–542. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2013.07.008

22. Fetzner MG, Asmundson GJG, Carey C, et al. How do elements of a reduced capacity to withstand uncertainty relate to the severity of health anxiety? Cogn Behav Ther. 2014;43(3):262–274. doi:10.1080/16506073.2014.929170

23. Hamonniere T, Varescon I. Metacognitive beliefs in addictive behaviours: a systematic review. Addict Behav. 2018;85:51–63. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2018.05.018

24. Brown M, Robinson L, Campione GC, Wuensch K, Hildebrandt T, Micali N. Intolerance of uncertainty in eating disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2017;25(5):329–343. doi:10.1002/erv.2523

25. Kraemer KM, McLeish AC, O’Bryan EM. The role of intolerance of uncertainty in terms of alcohol use motives among college students. Addict Behav. 2015;42:162–166. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.11.033

26. Kardefelt-Winther D. A conceptual and methodological critique of internet addiction research: towards a model of compensatory internet use. Comput Human Behav. 2014;31(1):351–354. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2013.10.059

27. Liu Y, Ni X, Niu G. Perceived stress and short-form video application addiction: a moderated mediation model. Front Psychol. 2021;12:1–8. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2021.747656

28. Reizer A, Galperin BL, Chavan M, Behl A, Pereira V. Examining the relationship between fear of COVID-19, intolerance for uncertainty, and cyberloafing: a mediational model. J Bus Res. 2022;145:660–670. doi:10.1016/j.jbusres.2022.03.037

29. Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. J Health Soc Behav. 1983;24(4):385–396. doi:10.2307/2136404

30. Demirtas AS, Yildiz B. Hopelessness and perceived stress: the mediating role of cognitive flexibility and intolerance of uncertainty. Dusunen Adam. 2019;32(3):259–267.

31. Carleton RN. The intolerance of uncertainty construct in the context of anxiety disorders: theoretical and practical perspectives. Expert Rev Neurother. 2012;12(8):937–947. doi:10.1586/ern.12.82

32. Dugas MJ, Hedayati M, Karavidas A, Buhr K, Francis K, Phillips NA. Intolerance of uncertainty and information processing: evidence of biased recall and interpretations. Cognit Ther Res. 2005;29(1):57–70. doi:10.1007/s10608-005-1648-9

33. Palma EMS, Reis de Sousa A, Andrade de Morais F, Evangelista Luz R, Freitas Neto ÁL, Lima PPF. Co** moderates the relationship between intolerance of uncertainty and stress in men during the Covid-19 pandemic. Revista da Escola de Enfermagem da USP. 2022;56:e103.

34. Rettie H, Daniels J. Coping and tolerance of uncertainty: predictors and mediators of mental health during the COVID-19 pandemic. Am Psychol. 2020;1–12. doi:10.1037/amp0000710

35. Busch PA, McCarthy S. Antecedents and consequences of problematic smartphone use: a systematic literature review of an emerging research area. Comput Human Behav. 2021;114:106414.

36. Gökçearslan Ş, Uluyol Ç, Şahin S. Smartphone addiction, cyberloafing, stress and social support among university students: a path analysis. Child Youth Serv Rev. 2018;91:47–54. doi:10.1016/j.childyouth.2018.05.036

37. Agnew R. Foundation for a general strain theory of crime and delinquency. Criminology. 1992;30(1):47–88. doi:10.1111/j.1745-9125.1992.tb01093.x

38. Liu Q, Zhang D, Yang X, et al. Perceived stress and mobile phone addiction in Chinese adolescents: a moderated mediation model. Comput Human Behav. 2018;87:247–253. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2018.06.006

39. Chen CY, Hong RY. Intolerance of uncertainty moderates the relation between negative life events and anxiety. Pers Individ Dif. 2010;49(1):49–53. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.03.006

40. Peng Y, Zhou H, Zhang B, et al. Perceived stress and mobile phone addiction among college students during the 2019 coronavirus disease: the mediating roles of rumination and the moderating role of self-control. Pers Individ Dif. 2022;185:111222. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2021.111222

41. Nolen-Hoeksema S. Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. J Abnorm Psychol. 1991;100(4):569–582. doi:10.1037//0021-843x.100.4.569

42. Pössel P. Can Beck’s theory of depression and the response style theory be integrated? J Couns Psychol. 2011;58(4):618–629. doi:10.1037/a0025092

43. Nolen-Hoeksema S, McBride A, Larson J. Rumination and psychological distress among bereaved partners. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1997;72(4):855–862. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.72.4.855

44. Treynor W, Gonzalez R, Nolen-Hoeksema S. Ruminative reconsidered: a psychometric analysis. Cogn Ther Res. 2003;27(3):247–259. doi:10.1023/A:1023910315561

45. Watkins ER, Roberts H. Reflecting on rumination: consequences, causes, mechanisms and treatment of rumination. Behav Res Ther. 2020;127:103573. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2020.103573

46. Bravo AJ, Kelley ML, Mason R, Ehlke SJ, Vinci C, Redman JC. Rumination as a mediator of the associations between moral injury and mental health problems in combat-wounded veterans. Traumatology. 2019;26(1):52–60. doi:10.1037/trm0000198

47. Genet JJ, Siemer M. Rumination moderates the effects of daily events on negative mood: results from a diary study. Emotion. 2012;12(6):1329–1339. doi:10.1037/a0028070

48. Jong-Meyer DR, Beck B, Riede K. Relationships between rumination, worry, intolerance of uncertainty and metacognitive beliefs. Personal Individ Differ. 2009;46(4):547–551. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2008.12.010

49. Davis RA. Cognitive-behavioral model of pathological internet use. Comput Human Behav. 2001;17(2):187–195. doi:10.1016/S0747-5632(00)00041-8

50. Brand M, Young KS, Laier C, Wölfling K, Potenza MN. Integrating psychological and neurobiological considerations regarding the development and maintenance of specific Internet-use disorders: an interaction of person-affect-cognition-execution (I-PACE) model. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;71:252–266. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.08.033

51. Sim M, Kim SY, Suh Y. Sample size requirements for simple and complex mediation models. Educ Psychol Meas. 2022;82(1):76–106. doi:10.1177/00131644211003261

52. Schoemann AM, Boulton AJ, Short SD. Determining power and sample size for simple and complex mediation models. Soc Psychol Pers Sci. 2017;8(4):379–386. doi:10.1177/1948550617715068

53. Wu LJ, Wang JN, Qi XD. Validity and reliability of the Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale-12 in middle school students. Chin Ment Health J. 2016;30(09):700–705.

54. Kwon M, Lee JY, Won WY, et al. Development and validation of a smartphone addiction scale (SAS). PLoS One. 2013;8(2):1.

55. Xiang MQ, Wang ZR, Ma B. Reliability and validity of Chinese version of the smartphone addiction scale in adolescents. Chin J Clin Psychol. 2019;27(05):959–964.

56. Lopez-Fernandez O. Short version of the smartphone addiction scale adapted to Spanish and French: towards a cross-cultural research in problematic mobile phone use. Addict Behav. 2017;64:275–280. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2015.11.013

57. Zhao H, Rafik-Galea S, Fitriana M, Song TJ. Translation and psychometric evaluation of smartphone addiction scale-short version (SAS-SV) among Chinese college students. PLoS One. 2022;17(5):e0278092. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0278092

58. Yang TZ, Huang HT. An epidemiological study on stress among urban residents in social transition period. Chin J Epidemiol. 2003;24(9):760–764.

59. Shen X, Wang J. Loneliness and excessive smartphone use among Chinese college students: moderated mediation effect of perceived stressed and motivation. Comput Human Behav. 2019;95:31–36. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2019.01.012

60. Han X, Yang HF. Chinese version of Nolen-Hoeksema ruminative responses scale (RRS) used in 912 college students: reliability and validity. Chin J Clin Psychol. 2009;17(5):550–551.

61. Nolen-Hoeksema S, Morrow J. A prospective study of depression and posttraumatic stress symptoms after a natural disaster: the 1989 Loma Prieta earthquake. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1991;61(1):115. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.61.1.115

62. Lei X, Zhong M, Liu Y, et al. Psychometric properties of the 10-item ruminative response scale in Chinese university students. BMC Psychiatry. 2017;17:152. doi:10.1186/s12888-017-1318-y

63. Steiger JH. A note on multiple sample extensions of the RMSEA fit index. Struct Equation Model. 1998;5:411–419. doi:10.1080/10705519809540115

64. Bentler PM. Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychol Bull. 1990;107(2):238–246. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238

65. Hu LT, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equation Model. 1999;6(1):1–55.

66. Liu C, Ren L, Li K, et al. Understanding the association between intolerance of uncertainty and problematic smartphone use: a network analysis. Front Psychiatry. 2022;13:917833. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2022.917833

67. Vujić A, Volarov M, Latas M, Demetrovics Z, Kiraly O, Szabo A. Are cyberchondria and intolerance of uncertainty related to smartphone addiction? Int J Ment Health Addict. 2023;2023:1–19.

68. Dhanalakshmi D. Perceived stress, cognitive distortion, sense of coherence and health among college students. Indian J Health Wellbeing. 2015;6(3):287–291.

69. Jensen D, Cohen JN, Mennin DS, Fresco DM, Heimberg RG. Clarifying the unique associations among intolerance of uncertainty, anxiety, and depression. Cogn Behav Ther. 2016;45(6):431–444. doi:10.1080/16506073.2016.1197308

70. Littlewood DL, Gooding PA, Panagioti M, Kyle SD. Nightmares and suicide in posttraumatic stress disorder: the mediating role of defeat, entrapment, and hopelessness. J Clin Sleep Med. 2016;12(3):393–399. doi:10.5664/jcsm.5592

71. Doğanülkü HA, Korkmaz O, Griffiths MD, Pakpour AH. Fear of COVID-19 lead to procrastination among Turkish university students: the mediating role of intolerance of uncertainty. BMC Psychology. 2021;9:1–8. doi:10.1186/s40359-021-00681-9

72. Doğanülkü HA. Life goals and proactive career behaviors: the mediating role of visions about the future and the moderating role of intolerance of uncertainty. Curr Psychol. 2024;43(1):72–84.

73. Hasan N, Bao Y. Impact of “e-learning crack-up” perception on psychological distress among college students during COVID-19 pandemic: a mediating role of “fear of academic year loss”. Child Youth Serv Rev. 2020;118:9.

74. Mittelmeier J, Cockayne H. Global depictions of international students in a time of crisis: a thematic analysis of Twitter data during COVID-19. Int Stud Sociol Educ. 2020;2020:1.

75. Chen JT, Lovibond PF. Intolerance of uncertainty is associated with increased threat appraisal and negative affect under ambiguity but not uncertainty. Behav Ther. 2016;47(1):42–53. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2015.09.004

76. Lyubomirsky S, Nolen-Hoeksema S. Effects of self-focused rumination on negative thinking and interpersonal problem solving. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1995;69(1):176–190. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.1.176

77. Chen JJ, Li HP, Yang YJ. Mediation effect of psychological resilience in the relationship between mobile phone addiction and mental health in college students. Mod Prevent Med. 2020;47(01):93–96.

78. Reeve KL, Shumaker CJ, Yearwood EL, Crowell NA, Riley JB. Perceived stress and social support in undergraduate nursing students’ educational experiences. Nurse Educ Today. 2013;33(4):419–424. doi:10.1016/j.nedt.2012.11.009

79. Lee SJ, Moon HJ. Effects of self-control, parent-adolescent communication, and school life satisfaction on smart-phone addiction for middle school students. Korean J Hum Ecol. 2013;22(6):587–598. doi:10.5934/kjhe.2013.22.6.587

80. Sahu M, Gandhi S, Sharma MK. Mobile phone addiction among children and adolescents: a systematic review. J Addictions Nurs. 2019;30(4):261–268.

81. Vanden Abeele M, Beullens K, Roe K. Measuring mobile phone use: gender, age and real usage level in relation to the accuracy and validity of self-reported mobile phone use. Mobile Media Commun. 2013;1(2):213–236. doi:10.1177/2050157913477095

Creative Commons License © 2024 The Author(s). This work is published and licensed by Dove Medical Press Limited. The full terms of this license are available at https://www.dovepress.com/terms.php and incorporate the Creative Commons Attribution - Non Commercial (unported, v3.0) License. By accessing the work you hereby accept the Terms. Non-commercial uses of the work are permitted without any further permission from Dove Medical Press Limited, provided the work is properly attributed. For permission for commercial use of this work, please see paragraphs 4.2 and 5 of our Terms.